This article should specify the language of its non-English content, using {{ lang }}, {{ transliteration }} for transliterated languages, and {{ IPA }} for phonetic transcriptions, with an appropriate ISO 639 code. Wikipedia's multilingual support templates may also be used.(June 2020) |
Lardil | |
---|---|
Leerdil | |
Pronunciation | [leːɖɪl] |
Region | Bentinck Island, north west Mornington Island, Queensland |
Ethnicity | Lardil people |
Native speakers | 65 (2016 census) [1] |
Dialects | |
Marlda kangka | |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | lbz |
Glottolog | lard1243 |
AIATSIS [2] | G38 |
ELP | Lardil |
Location of Wellesley Islands, the area traditionally associated with Lardil | |
Lardil is classified as Critically Endangered by the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger |
Lardil, also spelled Leerdil or Leertil, is a moribund language spoken by the Lardil people on Mornington Island (Kunhanha), in the Wellesley Islands of Queensland in northern Australia. [3] Lardil is unusual among Aboriginal Australian languages in that it features a ceremonial register, called Damin (also Demiin). Damin is regarded by Lardil-speakers as a separate language and has the only phonological system outside Africa to use click consonants. [4]
Lardil is a member of the Tangkic family of Non-Pama–Nyungan Australian languages, along with Kayardild and Yukulta, which are close enough to be mutually intelligible. [5] Though Lardil is not mutually intelligible with either of these, [6] it is likely that many Lardil speakers were historically bilingual in Yangkaal (a close relative of Kayardild), since the Lardil people have long been in contact with the neighboring Yangkaal tribe and trading, marriage and conflict between them seem to have been common. [7] There was also limited contact with mainland tribes including the Yanyuwa, of Borroloola; and the Garawa and Wanyi, which groups ranged as far east as Burketown. [8] Members of the Kaiadilt tribe (i.e. speakers of Kayardild) also settled on nearby Bentinck Island in 1947. [9]
The number of Lardil speakers has diminished dramatically since Kenneth Hale's study of the language in the late 1960s. Hale worked with a few dozen speakers of Lardil, some of these fluent older speakers, and others younger members of the community who had only a working or passive understanding. [10] [11] When Norvin Richards, a student of Hale's, returned to Mornington Island to continue work on Lardil in the 1990s, he found Lardil children had no understanding of the language and that only a handful of aging speakers remained; Richards has stated that "Lardil was deliberately destroyed" [11] by assimilation and relocation programs in the years of the "Stolen Generation". A dictionary and grammatical sketch of the language were compiled and published by the Mornington Shire Council in 1997, [12] and the Mornington Island State School has implemented a government-funded cultural education program incorporating the Lardil language. [13] The last fluent speaker of so-called Old Lardil died in 2007, [14] though a few speakers of a grammatically distinct New variety remain. [11]
Lardil has an intensely complex system of kinship terms reflecting the centrality of kin-relations to Lardil society; all members of the community are addressed by the terms as well as by given names. [15] This system also features a few dyadic kinship terms, i.e. titles for pairs rather than individuals, such as kangkariwarr 'pair of people, one of whom is the paternal great uncle/aunt or grandparent of the other'. [16]
Title | Relation(s) |
---|---|
kangkar | FaFa, FaFaBr, FaFaSi |
kantha | Fa, FaBr |
babe | FaMo, FaMoSi, FaMoBr |
jembe | MoFa, MoFaBr, MoFaSi |
nyerre | MoMo, MoMoBro, MoMoBrSoCh |
merrka | FaSi |
wuyinjin | WiFa, HuFa, FaFaSiSo, FaMoBrSo |
ngama | Mo, MoSi, SoWi, BrSoWi |
kunawun | WiMo, WiMoBr |
yaku | MoBrDaDa, sister (male ego), elder sister (female ego) |
kambin | Ch, BrCh (both male ego) |
karda | Ch, SiCh, WiFaSi, MoMoMo(and siblings) (all female ego) |
kernde | Wi, WiSi, 'second cross-cousin' |
kangkur | SoSo, SoDa (both male ego); BrSoSo, BrSoDa (both female ego) |
nginngin | SoCh (female ego), SiSoCh (male ego) |
benyin | DaSo, DaDa |
Traditionally, the Lardil community held two initiation ceremonies for young men. Luruku, which involved circumcision, was undergone by all men following the appearance of facial hair; [17] warama, the second initiation, was purely voluntary and culminated in a subincision ceremony. [18]
Luruku initiates took a year-long oath of silence and were taught a sign language known as marlda kangka (literally, 'hand language'), which, though limited in its semantic scope, was fairly complex. [19] Anthropologist David McKnight's research in the 1990s suggests that marlda kangka classifies animals somewhat differently from Lardil, having, for example, a class containing all shellfish (which Lardil lacks) and lacking an inclusive sign for 'dugong+turtle' (Lardil dilmirrur). [16] In addition to its use by luruku initiates, marlda kangka had practical applications in hunting and warfare. [20]
While marlda kangka was essentially a male language, the non-initiated were not forbidden to speak it. [20] Damin, on the other hand, was (at least nominally) a secret language spoken only by warama initiates and those preparing for second initiation, [4] though many community members seem to have understood it. [21] Damin, like marlda kangka, was phonologically, lexically and semantically distinct from Lardil, though its syntax and morphology seem to be analogous. [22] Research into the language has proved controversial, since the Lardil community regards it as cultural property and no explicit permission was given to make Damin words public. [21]
Death in Lardil tends to be treated euphemistically; it is common, for example, to use the phrase wurdal yarburr 'meat' when referring to a deceased person (or corpse). [16] Yuur-kirnee yarburr (literally, 'The meat/animal has died') has the sense 'You-know-who has died', and is preferable to a more direct treatment. [16] It is taboo to speak the name of a deceased person, even (for a year or so) when referring to living people with the same name; these people are addressed as thamarrka. [23] The deceased is often known by the name of his/her death or burial place plus the necronym suffix -ngalin, as in Wurdungalin 'one who died at Wurdu'. [23] Sometimes other strategies are used to refer to the dead, such as circumlocution via kinship terms. [23]
The consonant inventory is as follows, with the practical orthography in parentheses.
Peripheral | Laminal | Apical | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bilabial | Velar | Palatal | Dental | Alveolar | Retroflex | |
Plosive | p (b) | k (k) | tʲ (j) | t̪ (th) | t (d) | ʈ (rd) |
Nasal | m (m) | ŋ (ng) | ɲ (ny) | n̪ (nh) | n (n) | ɳ (rn) |
Trill or flap | r (rr) | |||||
Lateral | ʎ (ly) | l (l) | ɭ (rl) | |||
Approximant | w (w) | j (y) | ɻ (r) |
Lardil's consonant inventory is fairly typical with respect to Australian phonology; it does not distinguish between voiced and unvoiced stops (such as b/p and g/k), and features a full set of stops and nasals at six places of articulation. [24] The distinction between 'apical' and 'laminal' consonants lies in whether the tip (apex) of the tongue or its flattened blade makes contact with the place of articulation. [21] Hale's 1997 practical orthography has 'k' for /k ~ ɡ/ in order to disambiguate nasal+velar clusters (as in wanka 'arm' [25] ) from instances of the velar nasal phoneme /ŋ/ (as in wangal ‘boomerang’ [25] ) and to avoid suggesting /ɡ/-gemination in /ŋ + k~ɡ/ clusters (as in ngangkirr 'together' [25] ). The sounds represented by the digraphs 'nh' and 'ly' are not common in Lardil, but speakers perceive them as distinct, respectively, from /n/ and /l/, and they do occur in some words (e.g. minhal 'burnt ground', balyarriny [title of a social subsection]). [26]
Front | Central | Back | |
---|---|---|---|
High | i iː (ii) | u uː (uu) | |
Mid | e eː (ee) | ||
Low | a aː (aa) |
Lardil has eight phonemically distinct vowels, differentiated by short and long variants at each of four places of articulation. [27] Phonemic vowel length is an important feature of many Australian languages; minimal pairs in Lardil with a vowel length distinction include waaka/waka 'crow'/'armpit' and thaldi/thaldii 'come here!’/'to stand up'. [25] Long vowels are roughly twice as long as their short counterparts. [27] Some sources describe /e eː/ as low vowels, closer to /æ æː/.
Primary word stress in Lardil falls on the initial syllable, and primary phrase stress on the final word in the phrase. [28] These stress rules have some exceptions, notably compounds containing tangka 'man' as a head noun modified by a demonstrative or another nominal; these expressions, and other compound phrases, have phrase-initial stress. [29]
In addition to the common phonological alterations noted above, Lardil features some complex word-final phonology which is affected by both morphological and lexical factors. [31]
Augmentation acts on many monomoraic forms, producing, for example, /ʈera/ 'thigh' from underlying *ter. [31]
High vowels tend to undergo lowering at the end of bimoraic forms, as in *penki > penke 'lagoon'. [31] In several historical locative/ergatives, lowering does not occur. [31] It does occur in at least one long, u-final stem, and it coexists with the raising of certain stem-final /a/s. [31]
In some trimoraic (or longer) forms, final, underlying short vowels undergo apocope (deletion), as in *jalulu > jalul 'fire'. [31] Front-vowel apocope fails to occur in locatives, verbal negatives, many historical locative/ergatives, and a number of i-final stems such as wan̪t̪alŋi 'a species of fish'. [31] Back-vowel apocope also has lexically-governed exceptions. [21]
Cluster reduction simplifies underlying word-final consonant clusters, as in *makark > makar 'anthill'. [31] This process is "fed" in a sense by apocope, since some forms that would otherwise end in a short vowel arise as cluster-final after apocope (e.g. *jukarpa > *jukarp > jukar 'husband'). [31]
Non-apical truncation results in forms like ŋalu from underlying *ŋaluk, in which the underlying form would end in a non-apical consonant (i.e. one not produced with the tip of the tongue). [31] This process is also fed by apocope, and seems to be lexically governed to an extent, since Lardil words can end in a laminal; compare kakawuɲ 'a species of bird', kulkic 'a species of shark'. [31]
In addition to the dropping of non-apicals, a process of apicalization is at work, giving forms such as ŋawit from underlying laminal-final *ŋawic. It has been proposed that the process responsible for some of these forms is better described as laminalization (i.e. nawit is underlying and nawic occurs in inflected forms), but apicalization explains the variation between alveolar /t/ and dental /t̪/ (contrastive but both apical) in surface forms with an underlying non-apical, and does not predict/generate as many invalid forms as does the laminalization model. [31]
The first major lexical class in Lardil is its verbs, which may be subclassified as intransitive, transitive, and intransitive- and transitive complemented. [32] Verbs are both semantically and (as discussed below), morphologically distinct from nominals. [33]
Nominals are a semantically and functionally diverse group of inflected items in Lardil. Some of them are 'canonical nouns' which refer to items, people or concepts; [32] but many, the stative or attributive nominals, are semantically more like adjectives or other predicates. [32] Kurndakurn 'dry', durde 'weak', and other lexical items with adjectival meanings inflect exactly like other nominals. [34] Determiners (e.g. nganikin 'that', baldu(u)rr 'that (distant) west' [16] ), are also morphological nominals, as are inherently temporal and spatial adverbs [33] (e.g. dilanthaarr 'long ago', bada 'in the west' [16] ).
Lardil has a rich pronominal system featuring an inclusive-exclusive plurality distinction, a dual number and generational harmony. [35]
A 'harmonic' relationship exists between individuals of alternate generations (e.g. grandparent/grandchild); a 'disharmonic' relation is between individuals of consecutive or odd-numbered generations (e.g. parent/child, great-grandparent/great-grandchild). [36]
Harmonic | Disharmonic | |
---|---|---|
1 | ngada | |
2 | nyingki | |
3 | niya | |
1du exc. (11) | nyarri | nyaan |
1du inc. (12) | ngakurri | ngakuni |
2du (22) | kirri | nyiinki |
3du (33) | birri | nyiinki |
1P exc. (111) | nyali | nyalmu |
1P inc. (122) | ngakuli | ngakulmu |
2P (222) | kili | kilmu |
3P (333) | bili | bilmu |
Uninflected elements in Lardil include:
Nine basic inflectional endings appear on verbs in Lardil:
The future marker (-thur) indicates anticipation/expectation of an event, or, when combined with the particle mara, either the proposed outcome of a hypothetical (If you had done X, I would have Y’ed) or an unachieved intention; it also marks embedded verbs in jussive clauses. [39]
The (marked) non-future is used primarily in dependent clauses to indicate a temporal limit to an action. [40]
The contemporaneous ending marks a verb in a subordinate clause when that verb's referent action is contemporaneous with the action described in the main clause. [41]
The evitative ending, which appears as -nymerra in objective (oblique) case, marks a verb whose event or process is undesirable or to be avoided, as in niya merrinymerr 'He might hear' (and we don't want him to); it is somewhat analogous to English 'lest', though more productive. [42]
When one imperative follows another closely, the second verb is marked with a Sequential Imperative ending. [43]
Negation is semantically straightforward, but is expressed with a complex set of affixes; which is used depends on other properties of the verb. [44]
Other processes, which may be characterized as derivational rather than inflectional, express duration/repetition, passivity/reflexivity, reciprocality, and causativity on the verb. [45] Likewise, nouns may be derived from verbs by adding the suffix (-n ~ -Vn), as in werne-kebe-n 'food-gatherer' or werne-la-an 'food-spearer'; the negative counterpart of this is (-jarr), as in dangka-be-jarr (man+bite+neg) 'non-biter-of-people'. [46]
Lardil nominals are inflected for objective, locative and genitive cases, as well as future and non-future; these are expressed via endings that attach to the base forms of nominals.
The nominative case, which is used with sentence subjects and objects of simple imperatives (such as yarraman 'horse' in Kurri yarraman ‘(You) Look at the horse.') is not explicitly marked; uninflected nouns carry nominative case by default. [47]
The objective case (-n ~ -in) has five general functions, marking:
The locative marker (-nge ~ -e ~ -Vː) appears on the locative complement of a verb in plain form. [48] The objective case serves this purpose with negative verbs. [48] Locative case is formed by lengthening the final vowel in instances of vowel-final base forms such as barnga 'stone' (LOC barngaa). [48] While the Locative case can denote a variety of locative relations (such as those expressed in English by at, on, in, along, etc.), such relations may be specified using inherently locative nominals (e.g. minda 'near', nyirriri 'under') that do not themselves inflect for this case. Nominals corresponding to animate beings tend not to be marked with Locative case; Genitive is preferred for such constructions as yarramangan 'on the horse' (lit. 'of the horse'). On pronouns, for which case-marking is irregular, Locative case is realized via 'double-expression' of Genitive case: ngada 'I' > ngithun 'I(gen) = my' > ngithunngan 'I(gen)+gen = on me'. [48]
The genitive morpheme (-kan ~ -ngan) marks
The object of a verb in future tense (either negative or affirmative) is marked for futurity [50] by a suffix (-kur ~ -ur ~ -r), as in the sentence below:
Ngada
1SG(NOM)
bulethur
catch+FUT
yakur.
fish+FUT
'I will catch a fish.'
The future marker also has four other functions. It marks:
The instrumental case inflection is homophonous with the future marker, but both may appear on the same nominal in certain instances. [51]
The object of a verb in the (negative or affirmative) marked non-future also inflects for non-futurity. The non-future marking (-ngarr ~ -nga ~ -arr ~ -a) is also used to mark time adverbials in non-future clauses as well as the locative complement of a non-future verb. [52]
In addition to these inflectional endings, Lardil features several morphologically verbal affixes that are semantically similar to case markers ("verbal case") and, like case endings, mark noun phrases rather than individual nouns. Allative and ablative meanings (i.e. movement to or from) are expressed with these endings; as are the desiderative and a second type of evitave; comitative, proprietive and privative. [53]
Lardil nominals may also take one of two derivational (verbalizing) suffixes: the Inchoative (-e ~ -a ~ -ya), which has the sense 'become X', and the Causative (-ri ~ -iri), which has the sense 'make X Y'; other verbalizing suffixes exist in Lardil but are far less productive than these two. [54]
Reduplication is productive in verbal morphology, giving a non-future durative with the pattern V-tharr V (where V is a verb), having the sense 'keep on V-ing', and a future durative with V-thururr V-thur. [43]
In some instances nominal roots may be reduplicated, in their entirety, to indicate plurality, but Lardil nominals are not generally marked for number and this form is fairly rare. [55]
Given the rich morphology of Lardil, it is not surprising that its word order is somewhat flexible; however, the basic sentence order has been described as SVO, with direct object either following or preceding indirect object and other dependents following these. [56] Clitics appear clause-second and/or on either side of the verb. [56]
Unlike most other Australian Aboriginal languages, Lardil is a non-ergative language. [56] In an ergative language, the subject of an intransitive verb takes nominative case while the subject of a transitive verb takes ergative case (the object of this verb takes nominative case). In Lardil, subjects of both verb types are inflected for nominative case, and both indirect and direct objects marked for accusative [56] as in the following sentences:
Pidngen
woman+NOM
wutha
give
kun
EV
ngimpeen
2SG(ACC)
tiin
this+ACC
midithinin
medicine+ACC
'The woman gave you this medicine.' [56]
Kun, glossed as 'EV', is an eventive marker, marking a verb referring to something that actually occurred or is occurring.
Subjects (i.e. patients) of passive verbs also take nominative case, and their objects (i.e. agents), take accusative, [57] as in:
Ngithun
1SG(ACC)
wangal
boomerang+ACC
yuud
PERF
wuungii
steal+R
tangan
man+ACC
'My boomerang was stolen by a man.' [57]
Here, R is a maker of reflexivity.
Though part-whole relations are sometimes expressed using the genitive case as in (1) below, it is more common to mark both part and whole with the same case, placing the 'part' nominal immediately after its possessor nominal, as in (2). [58]
Ngada
1SG(NOM)
yuud-latha
PERF+spear
karnjinin
wallaby+OBJ
lelkin
head+OBJ
'I speared the wallaby in the head.' (lit. 'I speared the wallaby head') [58]
While very few speakers of Lardil in its traditional form remain, Norvin Richards and Kenneth Hale both worked with some speakers of a "New Lardil" in the 1990s which displays significant morphological attrition compared to the Old variety. [59] [11] Previously minor sentence forms in which the object of a verb takes nominative case have become generalized, even in instances where the verb is in future tense (objects of future verbs historically inflected for futurity). [59] One of a number of negation patterns has become generalized, and the augmented forms of monosyllabic verb roots reinterpreted as base forms. [60]
Akkadian is an extinct East Semitic language that was spoken in ancient Mesopotamia from the third millennium BC until its gradual replacement by Akkadian-influenced Old Aramaic among Mesopotamians by the 8th century BC.
A relative clause is a clause that modifies a noun or noun phrase and uses some grammatical device to indicate that one of the arguments in the relative clause refers to the noun or noun phrase. For example, in the sentence I met a man who wasn't too sure of himself, the subordinate clause who wasn't too sure of himself is a relative clause since it modifies the noun man and uses the pronoun who to indicate that the same "man" is referred to in the subordinate clause.
In linguistic typology, nominative–accusative alignment is a type of morphosyntactic alignment in which subjects of intransitive verbs are treated like subjects of transitive verbs, and are distinguished from objects of transitive verbs in basic clause constructions. Nominative–accusative alignment can be coded by case-marking, verb agreement and/or word order. It has a wide global distribution and is the most common alignment system among the world's languages. Languages with nominative–accusative alignment are commonly called nominative–accusative languages.
Suffixaufnahme, also known as case stacking, is a linguistic phenomenon used in forming a genitive construction, whereby prototypically a genitive noun agrees with its head noun. The term Suffixaufnahme itself is literally translated as "taking up of suffixes", which can be interpreted as the identical case marking of different but referentially-related phrases, with the presumption that nominal phrases possess a flat or non-configurational syntax. Across syntactic theories, case is seen as a bundle of features, and case agreement as the identity of case features. It was first recognized in Old Georgian and some other Caucasian and ancient Middle Eastern languages as well as many Australian languages, and almost invariably coincides with agglutinativity.
Mbula is an Austronesian language spoken by around 2,500 people on Umboi Island and Sakar Island in the Morobe Province of Papua New Guinea. Its basic word order is subject–verb–object; it has a nominative–accusative case-marking strategy.
Northern or North Sámi is the most widely spoken of all Sámi languages. The area where Northern Sámi is spoken covers the northern parts of Norway, Sweden and Finland. The number of Northern Sámi speakers is estimated to be somewhere between 15,000 and 25,000. About 2,000 of these live in Finland and between 5,000 and 6,000 in Sweden, with the remaining portions being in Norway.
The Nandi language, or Kalenjin proper, are a dialect cluster of the Kalenjin branch of the Nilotic language family.
The grammar of Old English is quite different from that of Modern English, predominantly by being much more inflected. As an old Germanic language, Old English has a morphological system that is similar to that of the Proto-Germanic reconstruction, retaining many of the inflections thought to have been common in Proto-Indo-European and also including constructions characteristic of the Germanic daughter languages such as the umlaut.
Hindustani, the lingua franca of Northern India and Pakistan, has two standardised registers: Hindi and Urdu. Grammatical differences between the two standards are minor but each uses its own script: Hindi uses Devanagari while Urdu uses an extended form of the Perso-Arabic script, typically in the Nastaʿlīq style.
Aguaruna is an indigenous American language of the Chicham family spoken by the Aguaruna people in Northern Peru. According to Ethnologue, based on the 2007 Census, 53,400 people out of the 55,700 ethnic group speak Aguaruna, making up almost the entire population. It is used vigorously in all domains of life, both written and oral. It is written with the Latin script. The literacy rate in Aguaruna is 60-90%. However, there are few monolingual speakers today; nearly all speakers also speak Spanish. The school system begins with Aguaruna, and as the students progress, Spanish is gradually added. There is a positive outlook and connotation in regard to bilingualism. 50 to 75% of the Aguaruna population are literate in Spanish. A modest dictionary of the language has been published.
Wagiman, also spelt Wageman, Wakiman, Wogeman, and other variants, is a near-extinct Aboriginal Australian language spoken by a small number of Wagiman people in and around Pine Creek, in the Katherine Region of the Northern Territory.
The grammar of the Marathi language shares similarities with other modern Indo-Aryan languages such as Odia, Gujarati or Punjabi. The first modern book exclusively about the grammar of Marathi was printed in 1805 by Willam Carey.
Faroese grammar is related and very similar to that of Icelandic. Faroese is an inflected language with three grammatical genders and four cases: nominative, accusative, dative and genitive.
Tundra Nenets is a Uralic language spoken in European Russia and North-Western Siberia. It is the largest and best-preserved language in the Samoyedic group.
Marra, sometimes formerly spelt Mara, is an Australian Aboriginal language, traditionally spoken on an area of the Gulf of Carpentaria coast in the Northern Territory around the Roper, Towns and Limmen Bight Rivers. Marra is now an endangered language. The most recent survey was in 1991; at that time, there were only 15 speakers, all elderly. Most Marra people now speak Kriol as their main language. The remaining elderly Marra speakers live in the Aboriginal communities of Ngukurr, Numbulwar, Borroloola and Minyerri.
The Ngarnji (Ngarndji) or Ngarnka language was traditionally spoken by the Ngarnka people of the Barkly Tablelands in the Northern Territory of Australia. The last fluent speaker of the language died between 1997 and 1998. Ngarnka belongs to the Mirndi language family, in the Ngurlun branch. It is closely related to its eastern neighbours Binbinka, Gudanji and Wambaya. It is more distantly related to its western neighbour Jingulu, and three languages of the Victoria River District, Jaminjung, Ngaliwurru and Nungali. There is very little documentation and description of Ngarnka, however there have been several graduate and undergraduate dissertations written on various aspects of Ngarnka morphology, and a sketch grammar and lexicon of Ngarnka is currently in preparation.
Zotung (Zobya) is a language spoken by the Zotung people, in Rezua Township, Chin State, Burma. It is a continuum of closely related dialects and accents. The language does not have a standard written form since it has dialects with multiple variations on its pronunciations. Instead, Zotung speakers use a widely accepted alphabet for writing with which they spell using their respective dialect. However, formal documents are written using the Lungngo dialect because it was the tongue of the first person to prescribe a standard writing, Sir Siabawi Khuamin.
Mẽbêngôkre, sometimes referred to as Kayapó is a Northern Jê language spoken by the Kayapó and the Xikrin people in the north of Mato Grosso and Pará in Brazil. There are around 8,600 native speakers since 2010 based on the 2015 Ethnologue 18th edition. Due to the number of speakers and the influence of Portuguese speakers, the language stands at a sixth level of endangerment; in which the materials for literacy and education in Mẽbêngôkre are very limited.
This article describes the grammar of the Old Irish language. The grammar of the language has been described with exhaustive detail by various authors, including Thurneysen, Binchy and Bergin, McCone, O'Connell, Stifter, among many others.
Turkmen grammar is the grammar of the Turkmen language, whose dialectal variants are spoken in Turkmenistan, Iran, Afghanistan, Russia, China, Uzbekistan, Tajikistan and others. Turkmen grammar, as described in this article, is the grammar of standard Turkmen as spoken and written by Turkmen people in Turkmenistan.