Sabine River Spanish | |
---|---|
Adaeseño, Adaesano | |
Pronunciation | [aðaeˈseɲo] , [aðaeˈsano] |
Native to | United States |
Region | Sabine Parish, Louisiana, Natchitoches Parish, Louisiana, Nacogdoches County, Texas |
Native speakers | (< 100 cited 1980s) [1] |
Early forms | Early Modern Spanish
|
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | – |
Glottolog | adae1234 Adaeseño Spanish |
The Sabine River is marked in lighter blue ■ on the right. The Neches River is marked in darker blue ■ on the left. |
Sabine River Spanish is a variety of the Spanish language spoken on both sides of the Sabine River between Texas and Louisiana. It has been spoken by a few communities descended from the 18th-century colonists who established Los Adaes and Nacogdoches. Due to its historical origins, it has a mostly conservative phonology with a vocabulary derived from rural Mexican Spanish. It is facing language death as it has not been passed onto children for several generations.
Sabine River Spanish was formed from rural Mexican Spanish, in spite of the common belief in Nacogdoches that the Spanish-speaking group around the Sabine River is of Isleño origin. [2] The Sabine River Spanish-speaking communities have no terms to identify themselves as a group. Adaeseño, in reference to Los Adaes, has been used by Armistead and Dr. Comfort Pratt for the dialect spoken on the Louisiana side of the river. Stark (1980) uses "Zwolle-Ebarb Spanish", from the names of two towns in Louisiana where it's spoken. Lipski uses the term Sabine River Spanish because the dialect extends to both sides of the river. [1]
The Sabine River Spanish communities were founded as part of a Spanish effort to settle the eastern edge of Texas and adjoining areas of Louisiana in the 1700s. Nacogdoches was founded as part of this settlement and so was Los Adaes.
The Spanish language was preserved in the Sabine River communities until the 20th century due to isolation and, in Texas at least, ethnic solidarity. The Louisiana communities had less ethnic solidarity but greater social isolation due to their distance from population centers, poverty, racial differences from the surrounding population, and the fact they spoke a "foreign" language. [3] The establishment of public schooling exerted strong linguistic pressure on these communities to learn and exclusively speak English, and the arrival of modern infrastructure such as electricity, paved roads, telephones, and the Kansas City Southern Railway through Zwolle reduced their isolation. [4] [5] This stopped the intergenerational transmission of Spanish, with most Spanish-speaking residents choosing not to teach their children the language. In this way the Spanish language has largely died out in a single generation along the Sabine River.
This dialect is currently moribund. As of the 1980s, there were no more than 50 individuals with significant active competence in Spanish on either side of the river. [1] Stark (1980) estimated the presence of just ten people who still speak Spanish fluently in the Zwolle-Ebarb area, who were mainly in their seventies and eighties. [5]
The Sabine River area's Spanish dialect is found on either side of the Toledo Bend Reservoir along the Sabine River. Most of the Spanish speakers in the Louisiana side were found around Zwolle, Ebarb and Noble, and in the Spanish Lake community near Robeline. In Texas they are concentrated in the Moral community west of Nacogdoches. [6]
The Louisiana and Texas communities differ in terms of ethnic identification. Louisiana residents have diverse appearances, some being very pale and others vary dark-complexioned, [7] [8] and have experienced a re-surfacing of American Indian identity. [9] As a result, they may identify ethnically as either Spanish, Indo-Spanish, or simply American Indian. [8] The Louisiana residents have been called "Meskin", "Chonche", and "Red Bones" by their Anglo-American neighbors. [9] Louisiana residents reject any identification as "Mexican", while Moral residents freely use the term mexicano and even occasionally call their dialect mexicano. [1] In Moral there is no identification with Native American culture, despite the open acknowledgement of many trigueño, or 'dark-complexioned' residents. [10]
Different studies and surveys have focused on different Spanish-speaking communities in the area. Stark (1980) focuses on the variety spoken in Zwolle and Ebarb; four of her five informants have lived most of their lives in Ebarb, with one later moving to Zwolle, while one lived most of his life near Zwolle. Pratt focused on all the Louisiana dialects, calling them Adaeseño. [11]
In terms of differences between the different varieties, Pratt (2000) finds that the Adaeseño varieties in Louisiana are generally homogenous. [12] Lipski (2008) says that the Moral dialect "may reflect some aspects of Mexican Spanish from the first decades of the nineteenth centuries" while the Louisiana dialects are derived from eighteenth-century Mexican Spanish. [13] This would be because Nacogdoches experienced a period of growth between 1821 and 1836. [14] The current Moral dialect has more speakers and is also more heavily influenced by modern Mexican Spanish, due to a higher frequency of contact with Mexican Spanish speakers. [15]
Gregory (1996) mentions a greater number of French loanwords in the speech of the communities closer to Natchitoches. [9]
Sabine River Spanish, being derived from northern Mexican Spanish, is rather phonologically conservative, generally retaining consonants and avoiding neutralizations. English influence is noted as well, and there are various phonological misidentifications, analogical forms and sporadic variations. [10] Sabine River Spanish is, like most Spanish dialects, yeísta, and like other Spanish dialects in the Americas, seseante. [16]
/s/ is occasionally aspirated or elided, with elision being more common than aspiration, though it is conserved most often. /s/ may even be aspirated or elided when between vowels. /s/ may also become voiced, like /z/, between vowels or at the end of a phrase. Before consonants, /s/ is often elided, and at the end of a phrase it's typically conserved. Nojotros or lojotros are common variants of nosotros 'we'. [10] [17] Stark (1980) reports that /s/ before /k/ and after a vowel is realized as [ ʃ ] in formal speech. Otherwise, /s/ is realized as [s]. [18]
The phoneme /f/ becomes a weak [h] before /w/, so afuera 'outside' is pronounced [aˈhwera]. [18] [19] Otherwise, /f/ is a voiceless labiodental fricative [f]. /x/ is typically pronounced [h] as well. [20] One speaker, again the oldest and most fluent in Spanish from Pratt (2000)'s survey, pronounced trajeron 'they brought' as [tɾuˈʃweɾon]. This allophone doesn't appear elsewhere in her survey. [21]
This variety does not velarize final -/n/, [10] though /n/ may occasionally be elided between vowels or at the end of a phrase. When it's elided, the preceding vowel is nasalized. [22]
The voiced palatal nasal, represented by ⟨ñ⟩, is typically pronounced as a nasal palatal approximant [j̃] which nasalizes the preceding vowel in informal speech, eg: año[ãj̃o] 'year', [23] [24] though Pratt (2000) failed to find this approximant pronunciation in the speech of her oldest, most fluent informant. [24] A similar pronunciation is found in Brazilian and Angolan Portuguese.
/m/ shows no irregularity. [19]
/t/ is occasionally alveolar, unlike the typical voiceless denti-alveolar plosive of Spanish, and may even be flapped. That is a result of contact with English. Unstressed vowels are often reduced to a schwa. [10] The other voiceless stops, /p/ and /k/, show little to no deviation from standard Spanish norms, nor does the affricate /tʃ/, spelled ⟨ch⟩. [25]
The lateral consonant /l/ is occasionally elided before other consonants. [26] In phrase-final and word-final position, elision of -/ɾ/ is relatively frequent, especially in verb infinitives. [10] [26] Word-final -/ɾ/ occasionally becomes /l/ before a word starting in a vowel. [26]
Lipski reports that the opposition between the alveolar trill /r/ and the alveolar tap /ɾ/ has been largely neutralized and that the extension of this neutralization points to an earlier origin. [10] [27] On the other hand, this neutralization isn't found in Stark (1980)'s notes. In Pratt (2000), the neutralization isn't found in the speech of the oldest, most fluent informant. [28] Pratt (2000) also finds that the trilled /r/ may occasionally be elided. [28]
In informal speech, /r/ can be elided before a denti-alveolar stop /t/ or /d/, or before a pause, thus: cardenal[kaðeˈnal] 'cardinal (bird)', carta[ˈkarta] 'letter', salir[saˈli] 'to leave'. [29]
The voiced obstruents /b/,/d/,/g/ show some deviation from standard pronunciation. /b/ may be pronounced as a fricative even at the beginning of a phrase or after a nasal. The labiodental fricative allophone [v], according to Pratt (2000), typically corresponds to a written, etymological ⟨v⟩, but it can be realized when pronouncing other words as well. [30] [31] /b/ is often elided when it's before another consonant, as in obtuvo[oˈtuvo] 'obtained'. It's also frequently elided in también 'also', typically pronounced [taˈmjen]. [31] /b/ is occasionally pronounced as a velar fricative [ɣ] when before [o] or [u]. [32]
/d/ is rarely realized as a voiced dental stop [d], even after a pause or a nasal. In general, it's realized as a voiced dental fricative [ð]. [33] Intervocalically, in an unstressed syllable, it may be elided, as in many other Spanish varieties, ie: dedo[ˈdeo]. [34] /d/ is frequently elided at the beginning of words, and donde 'where' is typically pronounced [ˈon.ne]. [35] It may also be realized as an alveolar tap [ɾ] between vowels, though this is only found among the last generation of Spanish speakers. [27] [36] In the sequence /ɾd/, either the /d/ or the /ɾ/ is often elided, thus guardan 'they save' is typically pronounced either [ˈgwaɾan] or [ˈgwaðan]. [37] In the sequence /dɾ/, the /d/ sometimes becomes an [i], thus padre 'father' and madre 'mother' are pronounced paire and maire respectively. [38]
/g/ is realized as a voiced velar stop after a pause and in any consonant cluster, for example in Goyo[ˈɡoʝo] 'Gregorio', algodón[algoˈðon] 'cotton', negrito[neˈgrito] 'black haw tree'. Otherwise, intervocalically, it's a voiced velar fricative [ɣ], [39] and it may also be realized as a fricative after a nasal, as in tengo[ˈteŋɣo] 'I have'. [40] /g/ is occasionally elided when between vowels, including after nasal vowels, as in tengo[ˈtẽo]. [40] /gw/ typically becomes /w/, thus guajolote[wahoˈlote] 'turkey'. [41]
The approximant /ʝ/, spelled ⟨y⟩ or ⟨ll⟩ is frequently elided in contact with /i/ and after /e/, for example gallina 'hen' becomes [gaˈina], silla 'chair' becomes [ˈsi.a] and sello 'stamp' becomes [ˈse.o]. [10] [42] One speaker, the oldest and most fluent in Spanish in Pratt (2000)'s survey, often adds an epenthetic [ ʝ ] between sequences of /i/ and /o/ or /i/ and /a/, as in tío[ˈti.ʝo] 'uncle'. [16] One speaker dropped [ j ] in the diphthong /ie/ after another consonant while speaking informally, saying [ˈrendas] for riendas 'reins' and [ˈtera] for tierra 'land'. He also dropped /ʝ/ after /i/ or /e/, thus saying [voˈtea] for botella 'bottle'. [41]
The vowel system in Zwolle-Ebarb contains the same 5 vowels as other Spanish varieties. Vowels are nasalized when they're between nasal consonants or before [j̃]. Additionally, /e/ and /o/ are typically mid vowels, [ e̞ ] and [ o̞ ], but they can be lightly raised after palatal sounds. [43] /e/ is often raised in many words, but it is not raised in word-final position, as is common in some other dialects. [44] Unstressed vowels, especially /a/, are often reduced to a schwa. [10] /o/ often becomes /u/, especially at the ends of words, and including in the conjunction o 'or'. [45] Hiatus between vowels tends to be avoided, either by the formation of diphthongs or by the deletion of some of the vowels involved. Also, the clusters /uar/ and /uer/ are frequently interchanged. [44]
Stark (1980) found that /a/ becomes nasalized before /o/, such as in the -ado ending where the /d/ has been elided. [43]
There is a tendency to simplify clusters and to drop consonants before voiceless stops in some words, as in doctor 'doctor', molcajete 'molcajete, and fuiste 'you went/were', pronounced dotor, mocajete, and fuite respectively. Additionally, word initial /e/ or /o/ can be dropped in sequences like /esC/ or /osC/, where C is a voiceless stop. Thus escuela 'school' is pronounced [ˈskwela], and oscuro 'dark' is [ˈskuɾo]. [46] Sometimes the entire first syllable of such words can be dropped, as in tar or cuela for estar, escuela 'to be, school'. [47]
Stark (1980) reports that the word-initial nasal is dropped in words starting with /njV/, so nieto 'grandchild' is realized [ˈjeto], although this was not found in Pratt (2000). [48]
The grammar of Sabine River Spanish reflects its origins in nonstandard, rural Mexican speech, as well as influence from English and morphological reduction due to language death. Archaic forms such as trujo/truje for trajo/traje 'brought', vido/vide for vio/vi 'saw', mesmo for mismo 'same', muncho for mucho 'a lot', and asina/ansina for así 'like this/that' are widespread. Many verb forms formed as a result of morphological leveling such as cierraron for cerraron 'they closed', dijieron for dijeron 'they said', cocinear for cocinar 'to cook', and tenimos for tuvimos 'we had' are common. [49]
Mexicanisms such as mero instead of mismo, like in Mexican Spanish, is common, also there's the expression ya mero for "almost". De nosotros 'of us' has almost completely replaced nuestro 'ours', as in some forms of Mexican and Caribbean Spanish. Nomás is frequently used instead of sólo or solamente, like in Mexican Spanish. Estar is very frequently used in place of ser. Que tanto and que tan are frequently used instead of cuanto or cuan. [49]
Dr. Comfort Pratt has found that Adaeseño, despite its mostly Mexican providence, uses vosotros as a second-person plural pronoun, [50] with the corresponding verb forms, as in vosotros tenéis 'you (pl.) have'. However, when tú is used alongside another subject, the corresponding verb form is that of ustedes, the third-person plural. Thus, tú y tu hermana tienen 'you and your sister have'. Voseo is nonexistent in Sabine River Spanish. [51]
P'atrás expressions are widespread, as in other Spanish varieties in contact with English. [49] As a result of language death and its speakers' greater fluency in English, gender and number agreement are greatly weakened. In addition, use of the subjunctive mood, the simple, or synthetic future tense, and the conditional tense is greatly reduced. The remaining speakers of Adaeseño generally prefer analytic constructions. [50]
Many Mexicanisms, including a large number of Nahuatl loanwords, and generally archaic or rustic words are used in Sabine River Spanish. [52] The majority of Nahuatl loans have to do with plants, animals, or elements of material culture. Almost all Nahuatl loans are nouns. [9] Sabine River Spanish has taken in very few English loanwords. [53] [54] French loans are common in the communities closer to Natchitoches, and they are more common than English words. [9]
Despite an extensive history of contact, Sabine River Spanish almost no loans from native American languages besides Nahuatl. This likely reflects frontier conditions in which native Americans were marginalized. [55] All words for "Indian" in this variety are at least partially derogatory, for example meco or chichimeco from "Chichimeca", the Nahuatl term for the "wild" tribes on Mexico's northern frontier. [54]
The term Chonche, a local slur for Spanish people, likely comes from the Wichita term for the Lipan Apache, many of whom were sold as slaves to the Spanish and French and were the ancestors of many Sabine River Hispanics, though it may have a Muskogean origin in a term for swallows. [54]
The term arrear, which refers to driving or spurring on animals, became the Zwolle-Ebarb community's term for driving a car. [43]
Some of the Nahuatlisms in Sabine River Spanish include:
Other Mexicanisms include:
Generally archaic words in Sabine River Spanish, no longer used in standard speech elsewhere, include:
Other items include:
Vestigial speakers of Sabine River Spanish, often with limited active competence in the language, would often engage in code-switching while attempting to speak entirely in Spanish. The rate of switching between languages in a single sentence was very high, and often violated the typical syntactic restrictions on Spanish/English code-switching. The speech of Adaeseños was, to Lipski, "impressionistically unlike anything I have ever heard from fluent Spanish-English bilinguals in any community." Code-switching could occur between subject pronouns and predicates, as in "they hervía las ollas" ("they would boil the pots"), and between negative words and the main verb, as in "si el papá y la mamá no agreed" ("if the father and the mother didn't agree"), or between fronted interrogative words and the rest of the sentence, as in "Nobody knows which way jueron" ("nobody knows which way they went"), to give some examples of code-switches that violate the normal syntactic restraints. [57]
In phonology, an allophone is one of multiple possible spoken sounds – or phones – used to pronounce a single phoneme in a particular language. For example, in English, the voiceless plosive and the aspirated form are allophones for the phoneme, while these two are considered to be different phonemes in some languages such as Central Thai. Similarly, in Spanish, and are allophones for the phoneme, while these two are considered to be different phonemes in English.
Spanish is the second most spoken language in the United States. Over 43.4 million people aged five or older speak Spanish at home (13.7%). Spanish is also the most learned language other than English, with about 8 million students. Estimates count up to 58.9 million native speakers, heritage language speakers, and second-language speakers. There is an Academy of the Spanish Language located in the United States as well.
Non-native pronunciations of English result from the common linguistic phenomenon in which non-native speakers of any language tend to transfer the intonation, phonological processes and pronunciation rules of their first language into their English speech. They may also create innovative pronunciations not found in the speaker's native language.
The different varieties of the Spanish language spoken in the Americas are distinct from each other as well as from those varieties spoken in the Iberian peninsula, collectively known as Peninsular Spanish and Spanish spoken elsewhere, such as in Africa and Asia. There is great diversity among the various Latin American vernaculars, and there are no traits shared by all of them which are not also in existence in one or more of the variants of Spanish used in Spain. A Latin American "standard" does, however, vary from the Castilian "standard" register used in television and notably the dubbing industry. Of the more than 498 million people who speak Spanish as their native language, more than 455 million are in Latin America, the United States and Canada in 2022. The total amount of native and non-native speakers of Spanish as of October 2022 exceeds 595 million.
The Andalusian dialects of Spanish are spoken in Andalusia, Ceuta, Melilla, and Gibraltar. They include perhaps the most distinct of the southern variants of peninsular Spanish, differing in many respects from northern varieties in a number of phonological, morphological and lexical features. Many of these are innovations which, spreading from Andalusia, failed to reach the higher strata of Toledo and Madrid speech and become part of the Peninsular norm of standard Spanish. Andalusian Spanish has historically been stigmatized at a national level, though this appears to have changed in recent decades, and there is evidence that the speech of Seville or the norma sevillana enjoys high prestige within Western Andalusia.
A digraph or digram is a pair of characters used in the orthography of a language to write either a single phoneme, or a sequence of phonemes that does not correspond to the normal values of the two characters combined.
The phonology of Portuguese varies among dialects, in extreme cases leading to some difficulties in mutual intelligibility. This article on phonology focuses on the pronunciations that are generally regarded as standard. Since Portuguese is a pluricentric language, and differences between European Portuguese (EP), Brazilian Portuguese (BP), and Angolan Portuguese (AP) can be considerable, varieties are distinguished whenever necessary.
Guttural R is the phenomenon whereby a rhotic consonant is produced in the back of the vocal tract rather than in the front portion thereof and thus as a guttural consonant. Speakers of languages with guttural R typically regard guttural and coronal rhotics to be alternative pronunciations of the same phoneme, despite articulatory differences. Similar consonants are found in other parts of the world, but they often have little to no cultural association or interchangeability with coronal rhotics and are (perhaps) not rhotics at all.
Some of the regional varieties of the Spanish language are quite divergent from one another, especially in pronunciation and vocabulary, and less so in grammar.
This article is about the phonology and phonetics of the Spanish language. Unless otherwise noted, statements refer to Castilian Spanish, the standard dialect used in Spain on radio and television. For historical development of the sound system, see History of Spanish. For details of geographical variation, see Spanish dialects and varieties.
French phonology is the sound system of French. This article discusses mainly the phonology of all the varieties of Standard French. Notable phonological features include its uvular r, nasal vowels, and three processes affecting word-final sounds:
Caribbean Spanish is the general name of the Spanish dialects spoken in the Caribbean region. The Spanish language was introduced to the Caribbean in 1492 with the voyages of Christopher Columbus. It resembles the Spanish spoken in the Canary Islands, and, more distantly, the Spanish of western Andalusia. With more than 25 million speakers, Spanish is the most widely spoken language in the Caribbean Islands.
A syllabic consonant or vocalic consonant is a consonant that forms a syllable on its own, like the m, n and l in some pronunciations of the English words rhythm, button and bottle, respectively. To represent it, the understroke diacritic in the International Phonetic Alphabet is used, ⟨U+0329◌̩COMBINING VERTICAL LINE BELOW⟩. It may be instead represented by an overstroke, ⟨U+030D◌̍COMBINING VERTICAL LINE ABOVE⟩ if the symbol that it modifies has a descender, such as in.
The phonology of Bengali, like that of its neighbouring Eastern Indo-Aryan languages, is characterised by a wide variety of diphthongs and inherent back vowels.
In phonetics and historical linguistics, fusion, or coalescence, is a sound change where two or more segments with distinctive features merge into a single segment. This can occur both on consonants and in vowels. A word like educate is one that may exhibit fusion, e.g. or. A merger between two segments can also occur between word boundaries, an example being the phrase got ya being pronounced like gotcha. Most cases of fusion lead to allophonic variation, though some sequences of segments may lead to wholly distinct phonemes.
Costa Rican Spanish is the form of the Spanish language spoken in Costa Rica. It is one of the dialects of Central American Spanish. Nevertheless, because the country was more remote than its neighbors, the development of this variety of Spanish followed a distinct path.
Guatemalan Spanish is the national variant of Spanish spoken in the Central American country of Guatemala. While 93% of Guatemalans in total speak Spanish, it is the native language of only 69% of the population due to the prevalence of languages in the indigenous Mayan and Arawakan families. Guatemalans typically use the second-person singular personal pronoun vos alongside the standard Spanish second-person singular pronouns tú and usted to form a three-level system of second-person singular address.
Wahgi is a Trans–New Guinea language of the Chimbu–Wahgi branch spoken by approximately 100,000 people in the highlands of Papua New Guinea. Like other Chimbu languages, Wahgi has some unusual lateral consonants.
Awankari is a dialect of the Hindko. It is spoken mostly in parts of Chakwal District in the north-west of the Pakistani province of Punjab.
The Choctaw-Apache Tribe of Ebarb is a state-recognized tribe and nonprofit organization in Louisiana. The members of the tribe identify as the descendants of Choctaw and Lipan Apache people and are required to prove lineal descent as part of their state-approved membership process. The tribe is based in the towns of Zwolle, Louisiana and Ebarb, Louisiana, both of which are in Sabine Parish, Louisiana, where the group have lived since the early 18th century.